Cross-Cultural Limitations in Erikson's, Piaget's, and Kohlberg's Theories
Shiraev, E. & Levy, D. (2021). Cross-cultural psychology: Critical thinking and contemporary applications. (7th ed. ). Routledge. THIS TEXT BOOK FOR CLASS PLEASE MAKE SURE YOU INCLUDE
Your responses must be supported and substantiated by the information discussed by the text authors. Outside sources may be used to support, not replace, the text material. Cite your work in APA format. All assignments MUST be typed, double-spaced, in APA style, and must be written at graduate level English.
Your response to each question should be 1- 1½ pages per question.
Your entire assignment should be 6-8 pages plus a title and reference page.
What cross-cultural limitations do you see in Erikson's, Piaget's and Kohlberg's theories?
Explain why people in non-Western cultures tend to “somatize” their distress, whereas people Western cultures have the tendency to “psychologize” it.
Explain and give examples of microaggressions
Compare and give examples of conformity in collectivist and individualist cultures.
Research shows, with some exceptions, that individuals from Western countries are more likely to display a strong internal locus of control than individuals from non-Western countries. Explain these findings.
What is the highest form of religious bonding? Describe it.
Activity Outcomes
Identify cognitive biases and their consequences in professional practice
Essay 11
Student’s Name
Institution
Course Code and Title
Instructor
Date
Introduction
Cross-cultural psychology examines the influence of cultural factors on human behavior, cognition, and development. This essay aims to explore various aspects of cross-cultural psychology, including the criticisms of Erikson's, Piaget's, and Kohlberg's theories, cultural responses to distress, microaggressions, conformity in collectivist and individualist cultures, the impact of culture on locus of control, and the highest form of religious bonding. Citing support from Shiraev and Levy’s text "Cross-cultural psychology: Critical thinking and contemporary applications" and additional scholarly sources, this essay conducts an in-depth discussion of these topics.
Cross-Cultural Limitations in Erikson's, Piaget's, and Kohlberg's Theories
Critics have cited various cross-cultural limitations in Erikson's, Piaget's, and Kohlberg's theories of development. These theories have significantly contributed to our understanding of human development, but their applicability to non-Western cultures is limited. According to Erik Erikson's theory of psychosocial development, individuals go through a series of stages, each characterized by a psychosocial crisis that they must overcome for progressive personality development. However, this theory was primarily developed based on observations of individuals from Western cultures. The emphasis on individual autonomy and independence in Western societies may not align with cultural values and beliefs in non-Western cultures, where collectivism and interdependence are prioritized (Apfelbaum, Norton, & Sommers, 2012). As a result, the manifestation and resolution of psychosocial crises may differ across cultures.
Similarly, Piaget's theory of cognitive development focuses on the development of logical thinking and reasoning abilities. However, Piaget's research was primarily conducted on Western children, which may limit its generalizability to non-Western cultures. Cultural variations in socialization practices and educational systems can influence the cognitive development of individuals and the acquisition of specific cognitive skills (Santrock, 2019).
Kohlberg's theory of moral development proposes that people develop through a series of stages, each characterized by different moral reasoning abilities. However, this theory has been criticized for its Western bias, as it was developed based on research conducted with Western participants (Turiel, 1983). Cultural variations in values, beliefs, and socialization practices can lead to different moral frameworks and priorities in non-Western cultures, challenging the universality of Kohlberg's stages of moral development. To overcome these cross-cultural limitations, researchers have called for the inclusion of more diverse samples in the study of human development.
By incorporating individuals from non-Western cultures, researchers can better understand the complexities of development across different cultural contexts (Apfelbaum et al., 2012). This can contribute to developing more culturally sensitive theories that better capture the diversity of human experiences and developmental trajectories. While Erikson's, Piaget's, and Kohlberg's theories have significantly influenced people’s understanding of human development, their cross-cultural applicability is limited. Cultural variations in socialization practices, values, and beliefs can influence the manifestation and progression of developmental milestones, challenging the universal validity of these theories. To address these limitations, it is crucial to incorporate more diverse samples in research to develop more inclusive and culturally sensitive theories.
Cultural Responses to Distress
In non-Western cultures, individuals "somatize" their distress, expressing psychological distress through physical symptoms such as headaches, stomachaches, or fatigue. This somatization can be attributed to cultural norms, where emotional expression is often discouraged or stigmatized. In contrast, individuals in Western cultures tend to "psychologize" their distress, emphasizing psychological explanations and seeking professional help for mental health issues. Western cultures place a greater emphasis on individualism, self-expression, and psychological well-being, which shapes their perception and response to distress. Moreover, cultural responses to distress can also be influenced by cultural beliefs and practices related to healing and healthcare. Non-Western cultures often have traditional healing practices that involve herbal remedies, rituals, and spiritual interventions. These practices are deeply rooted in cultural traditions and are seen as holistic approaches to addressing distress.
In contrast, Western cultures tend to rely more heavily on biomedical interventions and psychotherapy for addressing mental health concerns. The differences in cultural responses to distress highlight the importance of considering cultural context when providing mental health support. Mental health professionals working with individuals from non-Western cultures need to take keen attention to the somatization of distress and the cultural beliefs and practices that shape how individuals seek help and healing. Adopting a culturally sensitive approach that respects and incorporates these cultural perspectives and practices into the therapeutic process is crucial.
Cultural responses to distress vary across different cultures. Understanding these cultural variations is essential for mental health professionals to provide effective and culturally sensitive support. By considering cultural beliefs, values, and practices, mental health professionals can better understand and address individuals' unique needs and experiences from diverse cultural backgrounds.
Microaggressions
Microaggression is a pervasive form of discrimination that can profoundly impact individuals from marginalized groups. These subtle acts of bias, intentional or unintentional, occur in various forms and can be experienced through verbal, nonverbal, or environmental cues (Sue, 2010). One common type of microaggression is the use of derogatory comments. For instance, a person may make a racial or ethnic slur, perpetuating harmful stereotypes and reinforcing that certain groups are inferior (Sue et al., 2007). Such comments can profoundly affect the self-esteem and psychological well-being of the targeted individual.
Stereotyping is another form of microaggression that occurs when individuals make generalized assumptions based on someone's identity. For example, they assume that all women are emotional or that all Asian Americans are good at math (Nadal, 2011). These stereotypes can lead to exclusionary behaviors and limit opportunities for individuals to express themselves or reach their potential fully. Microaggressions can also manifest subtly, such as through nonverbal cues or microinvalidations. Microinvalidations involve dismissing or trivializing the experiences or perspectives of marginalized individuals. According to Sue et al. (2007), this can be seen when someone interrupts or ignores a person's contributions from a marginalized group during a discussion.
In the long run, the impact of microaggressions on individuals' well-being and sense of belonging should not be underestimated. Research has shown that experiencing microaggressions can lead to increased stress, anxiety, and depression among marginalized individuals (Nadal, 2011). These acts of discrimination perpetuate systemic inequalities and can contribute to a hostile and unwelcoming environment for those targeted. To address ...